All data is derived from the 2009 Latinobarómetro public opinion survey and covers Argentina, Chile, Paraguay, and Uruguay. Two independent variables were chosen to measure public evaluations of the economy, one measuring respondents’ evaluations of the national economy, and the other measuring respondents’ evaluations of their personal economic situation. A total of three variables were used to measure overt support for democracy, and a total of six were employed to measure intrinsic support for democracy. The intrinsic support variables mirror Inglehart and Welzel’s (2003) “self-expression values” of life satisfaction, interpersonal trust, tolerance of diversity, public expression, and liberty and participation.
The operationalization of life satisfaction and interpersonal trust is the same in this study as in Inglehart and Welzel. Tolerance of diversity is operationalized by tolerance of homosexual neighbors, the same indicator used by Inglehart and Welzel, except that in this study I have only employed this indicator, whereas Inglehart and Welzel combined it with a variable measuring tolerance of neighbors with AIDS to create a composite variable. The operationalization of public expression in this study is different than that of Inglehart and Welzel, as their indicator (responses to the question of whether respondents had ever signed a petition) was not asked in the Latinobarómetro survey in these countries and years.
Instead, respondents’ answers as to the importance they placed on voting were used as a substitute for the purposes of this study. I believe that this indicator accurately measures what Inglehart and Welzel intended, as it is a form of public participation and expression carried out through an institutional mechanism, just as is signing a petition. In a similar fashion, the variable operationalizing liberty and participation was measured in this study by respondents’ beliefs as to the importance of a free media. This parallels Inglehart and Welzel’s use of respondents’ beliefs as to the importance of protecting the freedom of speech, and was used because Latinobarómetro did not ask this question for these countries and years.
Lastly, a composite variable was created in order to combine all five self-expression values. This is of particular importance given that, as mentioned previously, Inglehart and Welzel (2006) found that all self-expression values tend to occur together. This composite variable therefore gives the most comprehensive view as to how these self-expression values may be associated with individuals’ evaluations of the economy.
Assuming the hypothesis of the previously mentioned literature that the existence of self-expression values are important to democracy, then the extent to which self-expression values and economic evaluations are associated may suggest important implications for democracy in the region, because a high association would likely indicate that permeation of self-expression values fluctuates with economic conditions. The potential volatility of the latter would then lead to volatility in the former, a condition that is clearly not conducive to democratic consolidation from a political culture perspective.
Analyses were conducted for each of the four countries individually (for each, N=1200), as well as all four countries combined (N=4800). The bivariate statistic gamma (G) was employed in order to measure the degree of association and directionality of the relationship between each of the dependent variables (overt support and intrinsic support for democracy indicators) and both independent variables (evaluations of national economic conditions and personal economic conditions).
All variables employed in this study are either ordinal or dichotomous, making the use of gamma appropriate. The use of gamma for the dichotomous variables is especially valid given that all of the dichotomous variables can be conceptualized ordinally.
The results of the analyses are detailed in Tables 1 to 5:
Table 1: Argentina
N=1200 |
Overt Support for Democracy |
Intrinsic Support for Democracy (Self-Expression Values) |
Overt Support #1 |
Overt Support #2 |
Overt Support #3 |
Life Satisfaction |
Interpersonal Trust |
Tolerance of Diversity |
Public Self-Expression |
Liberty and Participation |
Composite Variable |
Economic Evaluation (National) |
.050 |
.110** |
-.227*** |
.228*** |
.302*** |
.027 |
.157*** |
-.078 |
.212*** |
Economic Evaluation (Personal) |
.124** |
.078 |
-.125** |
.484*** |
.067 |
.060 |
.172*** |
.027 |
.268*** |
*=p<.05; **=p<.01; ***=p<.001
Table 2: Chile
N=1200 |
Overt Support for Democracy |
Intrinsic Support for Democracy (Self-Expression Values) |
Overt Support #1 |
Overt Support #2 |
Overt Support #3 |
Life Satisfaction |
Interpersonal Trust |
Tolerance of Diversity |
Public Self-Expression |
Liberty and Participation |
Composite Variable |
Economic Evaluation (National) |
.297*** |
.160*** |
-.079 |
.406*** |
.021 |
.115 |
.239*** |
.032 |
.275*** |
Economic Evaluation (Personal) |
.226*** |
.039 |
-.053 |
.500*** |
.045 |
.096 |
.200*** |
.029 |
.298*** |
*=p<.05; **=p<.01; ***=p<.001
Table 3: Paraguay
N=1200 |
Overt Support for Democracy |
Intrinsic Support for Democracy (Self-Expression Values) |
Overt Support #1 |
Overt Support #2 |
Overt Support #3 |
Life Satisfaction |
Interpersonal Trust |
Tolerance of Diversity |
Public Self-Expression |
Liberty and Participation |
Composite Variable |
Economic Evaluation (National) |
.154*** |
.208*** |
-.094* |
.273*** |
.094 |
.052 |
.087 |
-.132*** |
.095* |
Economic Evaluation (Personal) |
.082 |
.166*** |
-.069 |
.515*** |
.085 |
.079 |
-.027 |
-.103* |
.154*** |
*=p<.05; **=p<.01; ***=p<.001
Table 4: Uruguay
N=1200 |
Overt Support for Democracy |
Intrinsic Support for Democracy (Self-Expression Values) |
Overt Support #1 |
Overt Support #2 |
Overt Support #3 |
Life Satisfaction |
Interpersonal Trust |
Tolerance of Diversity |
Public Self-Expression |
Liberty and Participation |
Composite Variable |
Economic Evaluation (National) |
.331*** |
.071 |
-159** |
.273*** |
.168*** |
.076 |
.314*** |
.031 |
.267*** |
Economic Evaluation (Personal) |
.339*** |
.033 |
-.036 |
.513*** |
.143** |
-.016 |
.325*** |
-.047 |
.294*** |
*=p<.05; **=p<.01; ***=p<.001
Table 5: All Countries
N=4800 |
Overt Support for Democracy |
Intrinsic Support for Democracy (Self-Expression Values) |
Overt Support #1 |
Overt Support #2 |
Overt Support #3 |
Life Satisfaction |
Interpersonal Trust |
Tolerance of Diversity |
Public Self-Expression |
Liberty and Participation |
Composite Variable |
Economic Evaluation (National) |
.245*** |
.226*** |
-.226*** |
.289*** |
.212*** |
.130*** |
.244*** |
-.010 |
.254*** |
Economic Evaluation (Personal) |
.181*** |
.084*** |
-.061* |
.510*** |
.119*** |
.061* |
.180*** |
-.015 |
.262*** |
*=p<.05; **=p<.01; ***=p<.001
The first important note to make regarding the results is that none of the overt support or intrinsic support variables reached 0.6, the commonly accepted threshold for determining a strong association. Indeed, the highest associations across all countries occurred between life satisfaction and respondents’ evaluations of their personal economic situation, ranging quite close across all samples from .484 in Argentina to .515 in Paraguay. This association is not surprising, as a correlation between life satisfaction and, essentially, how much money one has, certainly possesses face validity. While only qualifying as a moderate association, this finding does suggest (especially given its consistency across all countries sampled) that the extent to which individuals are satisfied economically may also affect how satisfied they are with their lives more generally, and this life satisfaction is an important component to democratic culture if we accept Inglehart and Welzel’s findings. However, the association between evaluations of the national economy and life satisfaction is not nearly as cohesive, ranging from a low of .228 in Argentina to a high of .406 in Chile, and an average of .289 in all four countries combined. This suggests that life satisfaction is more greatly associated with personal economic conditions than national economic conditions, and that the relationship between this latter variable and life satisfaction is generally weak.
A focus on the overt support variables reveals a few interesting trends. First, there is a great degree of volatility regarding the degree to which each of the three overt support variables are associated with the two economic variables. In Argentina, the overt support #3 variable had the greatest degree of association of all other variables, and in fact reduced more than twice as much error in predicting evaluations of the national economy than did the next highest ranking variable.
Nevertheless, its association remains a weak one. In Chile and Uruguay, the overt support #1 variable was the strongest predictor of both the independent variables, even reaching a moderate level of association in Uruguay. In these countries, the overt support #3 variable reduced prediction error only by trivial amounts, often even failing to achieve statistical significance. Further complicating one’s ability to find a coherent trend is that in Paraguay, the overt support #2 variable achieved the greatest association with both independent variables, yet was often statistically insignificant in the other countries sampled.
Returning to the results for the intrinsic support for democracy variables (that is, the self-expression values), very few consistent associations are present, let alone strong ones, except for the previously mentioned association between life satisfaction and personal economic evaluation. One consistency is that the coefficients for the tolerance of diversity variable were very small and failed to reach statistical significance in all countries. It did reach statistical significance when all countries’ data were pooled together, although this is likely due the very high number of cases in the sample, and even here the association is very weak. The liberty and participation variable also was consistently insignificant, except for in Paraguay, although here the association is again only very slight.
Interpersonal trust coefficients are highly volatile among the samples. Values range from being statistically insignificant in Paraguay and Chile, to weak positive associations in Uruguay, to a (albeit barely) moderate, positive association (.302) in Argentina when linked with evaluations of the national economy. However, when this variable was compared with evaluations of personal economic conditions in Argentina, its value plummeted dramatically and became statistically insignificant. The sample of all countries pooled together shows a weak association with interpersonal trust and both independent variables, with its association with national economic evaluations being a bit stronger than with personal economic evaluations.
Public self-expression was also very volatile across all countries, ranging from statistical insignificance in Paraguay, to weak associations in Argentina and Chile, to moderate associations in Uruguay with both independent variables. In all of these cases, values were similar for associations with both of the independent variables.
The composite variable, measuring all five self-expression values simultaneously, is of particular importance given that Inglehart and Welzel (2006) found that all self-expression values tend to occur together. The results indicate that the composite variable reduced error in predicting both independent variables by between about 21 and 30 percent, with the exception of Paraguay, where error was only reduced by 9.5 (when crossed with national economic evaluations) and 15.4 percent (personal economic evaluations). In all instances, the association was slightly stronger with personal economic evaluations than with national economic evaluations.
However, these findings suggest that self-expression values are only weakly associated with evaluations of economic conditions (national and personal), as none of the coefficients passed the 0.3 threshold for declaring a moderate association. At the same time, these results also suggest that self-expression values, when pooled together under a single variable, are more consistently associated with national and personal economic evaluations across countries than are indicators for overt support for democracy.
However, while more consistent, the strength of this association is not greater on average than is the strength of the association between overt support indicators and economic evaluations. Indeed, the coefficients for the highest overt support democracy variable for a given country were generally very close to the coefficients for composite self-expression values variable.Continued on Next Page »
Allen, Leah. 2009. “Will a Bad Economy Hurt Democracy? Evidence from the AmericasBarometer Survey.” Inter-American Dialogue. http://www.thedialogue.org/page.cfm?pageID=32&pubID=1941 (20 March 2012).
Carlin, Ryan E. 2006. “The Socioeconomic Roots of Support for Democracy and the Quality of Democracy in Latin America.” Revista de Ciencia Política 26: 48-66.
Chu, Yun-han, et al. 2008. “Public Opinion and Democratic Legitimacy.” Journal of Democracy 19: 74-87.
Claassen, Christopher and Robert Mattes. 2007. “Does Support for Democracy Matter? Regime Preference, Democracy, and Democratization.” Presented at the Midwest political science Association Annual Meeting.
Dalton, Russell. 1994. “Communists and Democrats: Democratic Attitudes in the Two Germanies.” British Journal of political science 24: 469-93.
Graham, Carol and Sandip Sukhtankar. 2004. “Does Economic Crisis Reduce Support for Markets and Democracy in Latin America? Some Evidence from Surveys of Public Opinion and Well Being.” Journal of Latin American Studies 36: 349-77.
Inglehart, Ronald and Christian Welzel. 2003. “Political Culture and Democracy: Analyzing Cross-Level Linkages.” Comparative Politics 36: 61-79.
Inglehart, Ronald and Christian Welzel. 2006. “Public Support for Democracy: A Core Component of Human Development.” Presented at the UNDP-LAPOP Workshop, Vanderbilt University.
Lagos, Marta. 2003. “Latin America’s Lost Illusions: A Road with No Return?” Journal of Democracy 14: 163-73.
Latinobarómetro 2009 Data File, Released October 2011. Latinobarómetro Corporation.
Lipset, Seymour Martin. 1959. “Some Social Requisites of Democracy: Economic Development and Political Legitimacy.” American political science Review 53: 69-105.
Lipset, Seymour Martin. 1960. Political Man: The Social Bases of Politics. New York: Doubleday.
Przeworski, Adam. 1991. Democracy and the Market: Political and Economic Reforms in Eastern Europe and Latin America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Siegelman, Lee. 2006. “Top Twenty Commentaries: The American political science Review Citation Classics.” American political science Review 100: 667-687.
Shyu, Huoyan. 2007. “Does Economic Performance Matter? Economic Evaluations and Support for Democracy in Seven Asian Countries.” Presented at the Asian Barometer’s International Conference “Why Asians Support Democracy and Why Not?”
Wucherpfennig, Julian and Franziska Deutsch. 2009. “Modernization and Democracy: Theories and Evidence Revisited.” Living Reviews in Democracy: 1-7.
Endnotes
1.) This was the most recent year for which data was available to the public gratuitously.
2.) Given that my study will focus on public perceptions of economic conditions as opposed to the much broader focus of economic development that is characteristic of modernization theory, I will not address this literature in depth here. See Wucherpfennig (2009) for a comprehensive summary of research developments in this arena.
3.) They define effective democracy as the extent to which elites respect the rule of law insofar as they do not violate the political and civil freedoms of citizens. Countries were scored based on their Freedom House index scores and their Corruption Index scores from Transparency International. For a detailed explanation of the creation of this composite index see Inglehart and Welzel (2003: 66-68).
4.) For a more detailed discussion of these studies, see Chu et al. (2008).
5.) While a time-series analysis would be an insightful means at examining the relationship that this study intends to measure, such an analysis is unfortunately not feasible for this sub-region given that the Latinobarómetro survey did not consistently ask many of the questions used in this study (particularly the intrinsic support variables) in all of these countries across various years. It is also for this reason that this study must rely on individual-level survey data, as opposed to survey data aggregated across countries and years. That is, the lack of consistent survey questions across country-years limits the amount of available cases for such an analysis, and would therefore present degrees of freedom issues.
6.) See Appendix II for a complete list of all of the survey questions employed in this analysis.
7.) See note 6.
8.) Inglehart and Welzel use tolerance of homosexuality because as a concept it is more stable and translates better across countries than do, for example, race and ethnicity, which are highly fluid and interpreted differently in different societies. It is for this same reason that I employ it here.
9.) This composite variable was created by dividing each case’s value on each variable by the highest value possible on each respective variable. These quotients (five in total for each case) were then added together, and this sum was multiplied by 4 in order to eliminate decimals. The result was a scale that ranged from a score of 6 (highest degree of self-expression values) to 20 (lowest degree of self expression values). These scores were then converted to a 3-tier ordinal ranking: (1) 6-10, (2) 11-15, (3) 16-20. Due to the need to eliminate cases that did not have values for one or more of the self-expression values questions for the construction of the composite variable, N for this variable is less than N for the other variables, and is as follows: Argentina (1158), Chile (1117), Paraguay (1131), Uruguay (1154), all countries (4560).
10.) The variables labeled overt support 1, 2, and 3 refer to the overt support for democracy questions in the order they appear in Appendix II.
11.) In this paper, “strong association” refers to gamma coefficients of 0.6 and above, “moderate association,” 0.3 to 0.6, and “weak association,” 0 to 0.3. The same classification scheme is used for negative coefficients.
12.) The coefficients for the overt support #3 variables are negative because this variable coded responses of “under no circumstances would support a military government” as 2, as opposed to the other overt support variables, where responses most favorable to democracy were coded as 1. Therefore the negative relationship is what one would expect; as support for a military government decreases, evaluations of economic conditions increases.
Appendix
INDEPENDENT VARIABLES
In general, how would you describe the present economic situation of the country? Would you say that it is:
- Very good
- Good
- About average
- Bad
- Very bad
In general, how would you describe your present economic situation and that of your family? Would you say that it is very good, good, about average, bad or very bad?
- Very good
- Good
- About average
- Bad
- Very bad
DEPENDENT VARIABLES: OVERT SUPPORT
Which of the following statements do you agree with most? (Overt Support #1)
- Democracy is preferable to any other kind of government
- Under some circumstances, an authoritarian government can be preferable to a democratic one
- For people like me, it does not matter whether we have a democratic or a non-democratic regime
Do you strongly agree (1), agree (2), disagree (3) or strongly disagree (4) with the following statement: Democracy may have problems but it is the best system of government. (Overt Support #2)
- Strongly agree
- Agree
- Disagree
- Strongly disagree
Would you support a military government to replace the democratic government if the situation got very bad, or would you never support a military government under any circumstances? (Overt Support #3)
- Would support a military government if the situation got very bad
- Under no circumstances would support a military government
DEPENDENT VARIABLES: INTRINSIC SUPPORT
A. Life Satisfaction
In general, would you say that you are satisfied with your life? Would you say that you are...
- Very satisfied
- Fairly satisfied
- Not very satisfied
- Not at all satisfied
B. Public Expression
Some people say that the way you vote can change the way things will be in the future. Others say that no matter how you vote, things will not improve in the future. Which statement is closest to your way of thinking?
- The way you vote can change the way things will be in the future
- No matter how you vote, things will not improve in the future
C. Tolerance of Diversity
In this list you will see various groups of people. Could you select any that you would not like to have as neighbors?
A. Homosexuals
- Not Mentioned
- Mentioned
D. Interpersonal Trust
Generally speaking, would you say that you can trust most people, or that you can never be too careful when dealing with others?
- You can trust most people
- You can never be too careful when dealing with others
E. Liberty and Participation
Do you strongly agree (1), agree (2), disagree (3) or strongly disagree (4) with the following statement: The media must be able to publish news without being afraid of being closed.
- Strongly agree
- Agree
- Disagree
- Strongly disagree
Save Citation » (Works with EndNote, ProCite, & Reference Manager)
APA 6th
Elsen, J. R. (2012). "Public Opinion, Democracy, and the Economy: Case Studies from the Southern Cone." Inquiries Journal/Student Pulse, 4(08). Retrieved from http://www.inquiriesjournal.com/a?id=681
MLA
Elsen, Jacob R. "Public Opinion, Democracy, and the Economy: Case Studies from the Southern Cone." Inquiries Journal/Student Pulse 4.08 (2012). <http://www.inquiriesjournal.com/a?id=681>
Chicago 16th
Elsen, Jacob R. 2012. Public Opinion, Democracy, and the Economy: Case Studies from the Southern Cone. Inquiries Journal/Student Pulse 4 (08), http://www.inquiriesjournal.com/a?id=681
Harvard
ELSEN, J. R. 2012. Public Opinion, Democracy, and the Economy: Case Studies from the Southern Cone. Inquiries Journal/Student Pulse [Online], 4. Available: http://www.inquiriesjournal.com/a?id=681